• Bibliografía

    Libros:

    • Barlow, H. B. (1960). The coding of sensory messages. Current problems in animal behavior. W. H. Thorpe and G. J. Mitchison. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
    • Laming, D. (1968). Information theory of choice-reaction times. New York, Academic Press.
    • Peter Dayan and L. F. Abbott , Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems
    • Fred Rieke, David Warland, Rob deRuytervanSteveninck, and William Bialek (1999) Spikes: Exploring the Neural Code (Computational Neuroscience)
    • Eugene M. Izhikevich (2006) Dynamical Systems in Neuroscience: The Geometry of Excitability and Bursting
    • Edmund T. Rolls and Gustavo Deco (2001) Computational Neuroscience of Vision
    • Christof Koch (1998) Biophysics of Computation: Information Processing in Single Neurons (Computational Neuroscience)
    • David J. C. MacKay (2002) Information Theory, Inference & Learning Algorithms
    • Claude E Shannon and Warren Weaver (1963) The Mathematical Theory of Communication

    Artículos relacionados:

    • Altmann, C. F., H. H. Bulthoff, et al. (2003). "Perceptual organization of local elements into global shapes in the human visual cortex." Curr Biol 13(4): 342-9.
    • Andersen, R. A. (1995). "Encoding of intention and spatial location in the posterior parietal cortex." Cereb Cortex 5(5): 457-69.
    • Atick, J. and A. N. Redlich (1992). "What does the retina know about natural scenes." Neural Computation(4): 196-210.
    • Baddeley, A., H. Emslie, et al. (1998). "Random generation and the executive control of working memory." Q J Exp Psychol A 51(4): 819-52.
    • Barenholtz, E., E. H. Cohen, et al. (2003). "Detection of change in shape: an advantage for concavities." Cognition 89(1): 1-9.
    • Barlow, H. B. (1972). "Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology?" Perception 1(4): 371-394.
    • Barlow, H. B. and P. Foldiak (1989). Adaptation and Decorrelation in the Cortex. The Computing Neuron. C. Miall, R. M. Durbin and G. J. Mitchison. Addison-Wesley.
    • Barlow, H. B., T. P. Kaushal, et al. (1989). "Finding minimum entropy codes." Neural Computation 1: 412-423.
    • Botvinick, M., L. E. Nystrom, et al. (1999). "Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex." Nature 402(6758): 179-81.
    • Britten, K. H., W. T. Newsome, et al. (1996). "A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT." Vis Neurosci 13(1): 87-100.
    • Broadbent, D. E. and M. H. Broadbent (1987). "From detection to identification: response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation." Percept Psychophys 42(2): 105-13.
    • Cecchi, G. A., M. Sigman, et al. (2000). "Noise in neurons is message dependent." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97(10): 5557-61.
    • Cohen, L. and S. Dehaene (2004). "Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area." Neuroimage 22(1): 466-76.
    • Colby, C. L., J. R. Duhamel, et al. (1996). "Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area." J Neurophysiol 76(5): 2841-52.
    • Colby, C. L. and M. E. Goldberg (1999). "Space and attention in parietal cortex." Annu Rev Neurosci 22: 319-49.
    • Collins, A. M. and E. F. Loftus (1975). "A spreading activation theory of semantic processing." Psych.Rev. 82: 407-428.
    • Collins, A. M. and M. R. Quillian (1969). "Retrieval of time from semantic memory." Journal of Verbal Behavior and Verbal Learning 8: 240-247.
    • Corbetta, M. and G. L. Shulman (2002). "Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain." Nat Rev Neurosci 3(3): 201-15.
    • Changeux, J. P. and S. Dehaene (2000). "Hierarchical neuronal modeling of cognitive functions: from synaptic transmission to the Tower of London." Int J Psychophysiol 35(2-3): 179-87.
    • de Ruyter van Steveninck, R. R., G. D. Lewen, et al. (1997). "Reproducibility and variability in neural spike train." Science 275: 1805-1808.
    • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., L. Hertz-Pannier, et al. (2006). "Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103(38): 14240-5.
    • Dehaene, S. (2006). Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: Elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation. Attention & Performance 2006.
    • Dehaene, S., L. Cohen, et al. (2005). "The neural code for written words: a proposal." Trends Cogn Sci 9(7): 335-41.
    • Dehaene, S. and J. P. Changeux (2005). "Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: a neuronal model for inattentional blindness." PLoS Biol 3(5): e141.
    • Dehaene, S., M. Kerszberg, et al. (1998). "A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95(24): 14529-34.
    • Dehaene, S. and L. Naccache (2001). "Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework." Cognition 79(1-2): 1-37.
    • Dehaene, S., L. Naccache, et al. (1998). "Imaging unconscious semantic priming." Nature 395: 597-600.
    • Dehaene, S., C. Sergent, et al. (2003). "A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100(14): 8520-5.
    • Dehaene, S., E. Spelke, et al. (1999). "Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence." Science 284(5416): 970-4.
    • Dong, D. W. and J. Atick (1995). " Statistics of natural time-varying images." Netw. Comput. Neural Syst 6: 345-358.
    • Duncan, E. M. and C. E. McFarland, Jr. (1980). "Isolating the effects of symbolic distance and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: an additive-factors analysis." Mem Cognit 8(6): 612-22.
    • Duncan, J., G. Humphreys, et al. (1997). "Competitive brain activity in visual attention." Curr Opin Neurobiol 7(2): 255-61.
    • Edwards, W. (1965). "Optimal strategies for seeking information: Models for statistics, choice reaction times, and human information processing." Joural of Mathematical Psychology 41: 260-274.
    • Engel, A. K., P. Fries, et al. (2001). "Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing." Nat Rev Neurosci 2(10): 704-16.
    • Engel, A. K. and W. Singer (2001). "Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness." Trends Cogn Sci 5(1): 16-25.
    • Feldman, J. (2000). "Minimization of Boolean complexity in human concept learning." Nature 407(6804): 630-3.
    • Feldman, J. (2003). "What is a visual object?" Trends Cogn Sci 7(6): 252-256.
    • Feldman, J. and M. Singh (2005). "Information along contours and object boundaries." Psychol Rev 112(1): 243-52.
    • Field, D. J. (1987). "Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells." J.Opt.Soc.Am.A 4: 2379-2394.
    • Fodor, J. A. and Z. W. Pylyshyn (1981). "How direct is visual perception?: some reflections on Gibson's "Ecological Approach'." Cognition 9(2): 139-96.
    • Formisano, E. and R. Goebel (2003). "Tracking cognitive processes with functional MRI mental chronometry." Curr Opin Neurobiol 13(2): 174-81.
    • Geisler, W. S. and D. G. Albrecht (1995). "Bayesian analysis of identification performance in monkey visual cortex: nonlinear mechanisms and stimulus certainty." Vision Res 35(19): 2723-30.
    • Geisler, W. S. and R. L. Diehl (2002). "Bayesian natural selection and the evolution of perceptual systems." Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357(1420): 419-48.
    • Geisler, W. S., J. S. Perry, et al. (2001). "Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance." Vision Res 41(6): 711-24.
    • Gilbert, C. D., M. Sigman, et al. (2001). "The neural basis of perceptual learning." Neuron 31(5): 681-97.
    • Gold, J. I. and M. N. Shadlen (2001). "Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli." Trends Cogn Sci 5(1): 10-16.
    • Gottlieb, J. P., M. Kusonoki, et al. (1998). "The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex." Nature 391: 481-484.
    • Greenwald, A. G., S. C. Draine, et al. (1996). "Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation." Science 273(5282): 1699-702.
    • Itti, L. and C. Koch (2001). "Computational modelling of visual attention." Nat Rev Neurosci 2(3): 194-203.
    • Kapadia, M. K., M. Ito, et al. (1995). "Improvements in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys." Neuron 15(4): 843-856.
    • Khayat, P. S., H. Spekreijse, et al. (2004). "Visual information transfer across eye movements in the monkey." Vision Res 44(25): 2901-17.
    • Koch, C. and S. Ullman (1985). "Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry." Human Neurobiology 4: 219-227.
    • Koch, I. (2005). "Sequential task predictability in task switching." Psychon Bull Rev 12(1): 107-12.
    • Kourtzi, Z., L. R. Betts, et al. (2005). "Distributed neural plasticity for shape learning in the human visual cortex." PLoS Biol 3(7): e204.
    • Kourtzi, Z., A. S. Tolias, et al. (2003). "Integration of local features into global shapes: monkey and human FMRI studies." Neuron 37(2): 333-46.
    • Kovacs, I. and B. Julesz (1993). "A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: Effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation." Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.USA 90: 7495-7497.
    • Kovacs, I. and B. Julesz (1994). "Perceptual sensitivity maps within globally defined visual shapes." Nature 370: 644-646.
    • Laming, D. (1979). "Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times." Acta Psychol (Amst) 43(5): 381-412.
    • Lamme, V. A. and P. R. Roelfsema (2000). "The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing." Trends Neurosci 23(11): 571-9.
    • Lamme, V. A., H. Super, et al. (2000). "The role of primary visual cortex (V1) in visual awareness." Vision Res 40(10-12): 1507-21.
    • Lee, T. S. and D. Mumford (2003). "Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex." J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 20(7): 1434-48.
    • Lee, T. S., C. F. Yang, et al. (2002). "Neural activity in early visual cortex reflects behavioral experience and higher-order perceptual saliency." Nat Neurosci 5(6): 589-97.
    • Lee.T.S, Mumford.D., et al. (1999). "The role of primary visual cortex in higher level vision." Vision Res. 38: 2429-2454.
    • Leon, M. I. and M. N. Shadlen (1998). "Exploring the neurophysiology of decisions." Neuron 21(4): 669-72.
    • Li, Z. (1998). "A neural model of contour integration in the primary visual cortex." Neural Computation 10: 903-940.
    • Mazurek, M. E., J. D. Roitman, et al. (2003). "A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making." Cereb Cortex 13(11): 1257-69.
    • McCandliss, B. D., L. Cohen, et al. (2003). "The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus." Trends Cogn Sci 7(7): 293-299.
    • McIntosh, A. R. (2000). "Towards a network theory of cognition." Neural Netw 13: 861-870.
    • McIntosh, A. R. and F. Gonzalez Lima (1998). "Large-scale functional connectivity in associative learning: interrelations of the rat auditory, visual and lymbic systems." J.Neurophysiol. 80(6): 3148-3162.
    • Mumford, D. (1991). "On the computational architecture of the neocortex. I. The role of the thalamo-cortical loop." Biol Cybern 65(2): 135-45.
    • Mumford.D. (1991). "Mathematical Theories of Shape: Do they model perception?" Geometric Methods in Computer Vision 1570.
    • Peck, C. C., J. Kozloski, et al. (2003). "Simulation Infrastructure for Modeling Large Scale Neural Systems." Lecture Notes in Computer Science 2660: 1127-1136.
    • Pica, P., C. Lemer, et al. (2004). "Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group." Science 306(5695): 499-503.
    • Posner, M. I. and S. Dehaene (1994). "Attentional networks." Trends Neurosci 17(2): 75-9.
    • Prinzmetal, W. (1981). "Principles of feature integration in visual perception." Percept Psychophys 30(4): 330-40.
    • Prinzmetal, W. (1995). "Visual Feature Integration in a World of Objects." Current Directions in Psychological Science 4: 90-94.
    • Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). "Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception." Behav Brain Sci 22(3): 341-65; discussion 366-423.
    • Quillian, M. R. (1967). "Word concepts: a theory and simulation of some basic semantic capabilities." Behavioral Science 12: 410-430.
    • Quillian, M. R. (2001). Semantic Memory. Semantic Information Processing. M. L. Minsky. Cambridge, MIT Press: 227-270.
    • Ramachandran, V. S. and S. Cobb (1995). "Visual Attention Modulates Metacontrast Masking." Nature 373: 66-68.
    • Ratcliff, R., T. Van Zandt, et al. (1999). "Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time." Psychol Rev 106(2): 261-300.
    • Reinagel, P. and A. M. Zador (1999). "Natural scene statistics at the centre of gaze." Network 10(4): 341-50.
    • Roelfsema, P. R., P. S. Khayat, et al. (2003). "Subtask sequencing in the primary visual cortex." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100(9): 5467-72.
    • Roelfsema, P. R., V. A. Lamme, et al. (2000). "The implementation of visual routines." Vision Res 40(10-12): 1385-411.
    • Roelfsema, P. R., V. A. F. Lamme, et al. (1998). "Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey." Nature 395: 376-381.
    • Rolls, E. T., A. Treves, et al. (1997). "The representational capacity of the distributed encoding of information provided by populations of neurons in primate temporal visual cortex." Exp Brain Res 114(1): 149-62.
    • Ruderman, D. and W. Bialek (1994). "Statistics of natural images-scaling in the woods." Phys.Rev.Lett. 73(6): 814-817.
    • Salinas, E. and R. Romo (1998). "Conversion of sensory signals into motor commands in primary motor cortex." J Neurosci 18(1): 499-511.
    • Scholl, B. J., Z. W. Pylyshyn, et al. (2001). "What is a visual object? Evidence from target merging in multiple object tracking." Cognition 80(1-2): 159-77.
    • Sergent, C., S. Baillet, et al. (2005). "Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink." Nat Neurosci 8(10): 1391-400.
    • Shadlen, M. N. and W. T. Newsome (1996). "Motion perception: seeing and deciding." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93(2): 628-33.
    • Shulman, G. L., M. Corbetta, et al. (1997). "Top-down modulation of early sensory cortex." Cereb Cortex 7(3): 193-206.
    • Sigman, M. (2004). "Bridging psychology and mathematics: can the brain understand the brain?" PLoS Biol 2(9): E297.
    • Sigman, M. and G. A. Cecchi (2002). "Global organization of the Wordnet lexicon." PNAS 99(3): 1742-1747.
    • Sigman, M., G. A. Cecchi, et al. (2001). "On a common circle: natural scenes and Gestalt rules." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98(4): 1935-40.
    • Sigman, M. and S. Dehaene (2005). "Parsing a Cognitive Task: A Characterization of the Mind's Bottleneck." PLoS Biol 3(2): e37.
    • Sigman, M. and S. Dehaene (2006). "Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty." PLoS Biol 4(7): e220.
    • Sigman, M. and C. D. Gilbert (2000). "Learning to find a shape." Nature Neuroscience 3(3): 264-269.
    • Sigman, M., A. Jobert, et al. (2007). "Parsing a sequence of brain activations at psychological times using fMRI." Neuroimage.
    • Sporns, O., G. Tononi, et al. (2000). "Theoretical neuroanatomy:Relating anatomical and functional connectivity in graphs and cortical connection matrices." Cerebral Cortex 10: 127-141.
    • Srinivasan, R., D. P. Russell, et al. (1999). "Increased synchronization of neuromagnetic responses during conscious perception." J Neurosci 19(13): 5435-48.
    • Tononi, G. and G. M. Edelman (1998). "Consciousness and complexity." Science 282(5395): 1846-51.
    • Tononi, G., O. Sporns, et al. (1992). "Reentry and the problem of integrating multiple cortical areas: simulation of dynamic integration in the visual system." Cereb Cortex 2(4): 310-35.
    • Usher, M., Y. Bonneh, et al. (1999). "Mechanisms for spatial integration in visual detection: a model based on lateral interactions." Spatial Vision 12: 187-209.
    • Vinje, W. E. and J. L. Gallant (2000). "Sparse Coding and Decorrelation in Primary visual cortex during natural vision." Science 287: 1273-1276.
    • Westheimer, G. and E. J. Ley (1997). "Spatial and temporal integration of signals in foveal line orientation." J.Neurophysiol. 77: 2677-2684.
    • Wolfe, J. M., Cave.K.R, et al. (1989). "Guided Search: An alternative to the feature integration model of visual search." Jrn.Exp.Psych: Human.Percept.and.Perf. 15: 419-433.
    • Wong, K. F. and X. J. Wang (2006). "A recurrent network mechanism of time integration in perceptual decisions." J Neurosci 26(4): 1314-28.
    • Wurtz, R. H., M. E. Goldberg, et al. (1982). "Brain mechanisms of visual attention." Sci Am 246(6): 124-35.